Household interventions for preventing domestic lead exposure in children

Why is this review important?

Lead exposure is a serious health risk, especially for children. Lead poisoning at high levels can cause anaemia, multi-organ damage, seizures, coma and death in children. At chronic low levels, it can lead to cognitive (thought processes), psychological (mental and emotional states) and neurobehavioural impairment (e.g. aggression, hyperactivity). There are many potential sources of lead in the environment, therefore researchers have studied different educational and environmental household interventions to reduce lead exposure in children, such as parental education, removal of lead dust or home remediation work. However, it is not clear if and to what extent these interventions work in reducing or preventing further lead exposure in children.

Who will be interested in this review?

- Parents and carers who want to prevent domestic lead exposure in children.

- Health professionals and decision-makers who are interested in methods to prevent domestic lead exposure in children.

What questions does this review aim to answer?

We wanted to find out if educational or environmental household interventions, or combinations of both, are effective in preventing or reducing further domestic lead exposure in children up to 18 years of age. We were interested in looking at improvements in cognitive and neurobehavioural development, potential harms, reductions in blood lead levels and household lead dust levels.

Which studies were included in the review?

We searched databases up to March 2020 for randomised controlled trials (RCTs; where participants are randomly assigned, in this case, to one or more groups to receive the treatment and one group that does not) and quasi-RCTs (where children are assigned to groups using methods that are not strictly random). We found 17 studies (three new to this update), involving 3282 children from birth to six years of age. The studies investigated educational or environmental interventions, or a combination of both, to reduce domestic lead exposure in children. Children in all studies were under six years of age. Fifteen studies took place in urban areas of North America, one in Australia and one in China. Most studies were performed in areas with low socioeconomic status. Boys and girls were equally represented in the studies. The duration of the intervention ranged from three months to 24 months in 15 studies, and two studies performed an intervention on a single occasion. Fourteen studies used flawed methods that could distort their results, making them less trustworthy.

Follow-up periods ranged from three months to eight years. National or international research grants or governments funded 15 studies; two studies did not report their funding sources.

What does the evidence from the review reveal?

Educational interventions: none of the included studies in this comparison assessed effects on cognitive or neurobehavioural outcomes, or harms. Compared to no intervention, educational interventions probably result in no differences in blood lead levels of young children or floor dust levels (moderate-quality evidence).

Environmental interventions: one study comparing dust control measures with no intervention showed little to no difference in cognitive and neurobehavioural outcomes between the groups after three years to eight years. The same study assessed harms and found none associated with the intervention, but observed two children with side effects in the control group. All included studies in this comparison found that dust control did not lead to more or less reduced blood lead levels of young children (moderate-quality evidence) or floor dust levels than no intervention (very low-quality evidence). Two studies assessed the effect of soil abatement and did not allow any conclusions about its effectiveness (very low-quality evidence).

Combination of educational and environmental interventions versus standard education: there is insufficient evidence that combination interventions reduce blood lead levels or floor dust lead levels (very low-quality evidence), and further studies need to address this research gap.

What should happen next?

More research is needed to find out what is effective for preventing children's exposure to lead. Studies should be carried out in different socioeconomic groups in high-, middle- and low-income countries to consider how interventions work in contexts shaped by different levels of industrialisation or environmental and occupational health safety regulations.

Authors' conclusions: 

Based on available evidence, household educational interventions and environmental interventions (namely dust control measures) show no evidence of a difference in reducing blood lead levels in children as a population health measure. The evidence of the effects of environmental interventions on cognitive and neurobehavioural outcomes and adverse events is uncertain too.

Further trials are required to establish the most effective intervention for reducing or even preventing further lead exposure. Key elements of these trials should include strategies to reduce multiple sources of lead exposure simultaneously using empirical dust clearance levels. It is also necessary for trials to be carried out in low- and middle-income countries and in differing socioeconomic groups in high-income countries.

Read the full abstract...
Background: 

Lead exposure is a serious health hazard, especially for children. It is associated with physical, cognitive and neurobehavioural impairment in children. There are many potential sources of lead in the environment, therefore trials have tested many household interventions to prevent or reduce lead exposure. This is an update of a previously published review.

Objectives: 

To assess the effects of household interventions intended to prevent or reduce further lead exposure in children on improvements in cognitive and neurobehavioural development, reductions in blood lead levels and reductions in household dust lead levels.

Search strategy: 

In March 2020, we updated our searches of CENTRAL, MEDLINE, Embase, 10 other databases and ClinicalTrials.gov. We also searched Google Scholar, checked the reference lists of relevant studies and contacted experts to identify unpublished studies.

Selection criteria: 

Randomised controlled trials (RCTs) and quasi-RCTs of household educational or environmental interventions, or combinations of interventions to prevent lead exposure in children (from birth to 18 years of age), where investigators reported at least one standardised outcome measure.

Data collection and analysis: 

Two authors independently reviewed all eligible studies for inclusion, assessed risk of bias and extracted data. We contacted trialists to obtain missing information. We assessed the certainty of the evidence using the GRADE approach.

Main results: 

We included 17 studies (three new to this update), involving 3282 children: 16 RCTs (involving 3204 children) and one quasi-RCT (involving 78 children). Children in all studies were under six years of age. Fifteen studies took place in urban areas of North America, one in Australia and one in China. Most studies were in areas with low socioeconomic status. Girls and boys were equally represented in those studies reporting this information. The duration of the intervention ranged from three months to 24 months in 15 studies, while two studies performed interventions on a single occasion. Follow-up periods ranged from three months to eight years. Three RCTs were at low risk of bias in all assessed domains. The other 14 studies were at unclear or high risk of bias; for example, we considered two RCTs and one quasi-RCT at high risk of selection bias and six RCTs at high risk of attrition bias. National or international research grants or governments funded 15 studies, while the other two did not report their funding sources.

Education interventions versus no intervention

None of the included studies in this comparison assessed effects on cognitive or neurobehavioural outcomes, or adverse events. All studies reported data on blood lead level outcomes.
Educational interventions showed there was probably no evidence of a difference in reducing blood lead levels (continuous: mean difference (MD) –0.03, 95% confidence interval (CI) –0.13 to 0.07; I² = 0%; 5 studies, 815 participants; moderate-certainty evidence; log-transformed data), or in reducing floor dust levels (MD –0.07, 95% CI –0.37 to 0.24; I² = 0%; 2 studies, 318 participants; moderate-certainty evidence).

Environmental interventions versus no intervention

Dust control: one study in this comparison reported data on cognitive and neurobehavioural outcomes, and on adverse events in children. The study showed numerically there may be better neurobehavioural outcomes in children of the intervention group. However, differences were small and the CI included both a beneficial and non-beneficial effect of the environmental intervention (e.g. mental development (Bayley Scales of Infant Development-II): MD 0.1, 95% CI –2.1 to 2.4; 1 study, 302 participants; low-certainty evidence). The same study did not observe any adverse events related to the intervention during the eight-year follow-up, but observed two children with adverse events in the control group (1 study, 355 participants; very low-certainty evidence).
Meta-analysis also found no evidence of effectiveness on blood lead levels (continuous: MD –0.02, 95% CI –0.09 to 0.06; I² = 0%; 4 studies, 565 participants; moderate-certainty evidence; log-transformed data). We could not pool the data regarding floor dust levels, but studies reported that there may be no evidence of a difference between the groups (very low-certainty evidence).

Soil abatement: the two studies assessing this environmental intervention only reported on the outcome of 'blood lead level'. One study showed a small effect on blood lead level reduction, while the other study showed no effect. Therefore, we deem the current evidence insufficient to draw conclusions about the effectiveness of soil abatement (very low-certainty evidence).

Combination of educational and environmental interventions versus standard education

Studies in this comparison only reported on blood lead levels and dust lead levels. We could not pool the studies in a meta-analysis due to substantial differences between the studies. Since the studies reported inconsistent results, the evidence is currently insufficient to clarify whether a combination of interventions reduces blood lead levels and floor dust levels (very low-certainty evidence).